Qualitative Measures Of Verbal Fluency Psychology Essay

Print   

23 Mar 2015

Disclaimer:
This essay has been written and submitted by students and is not an example of our work. Please click this link to view samples of our professional work witten by our professional essay writers. Any opinions, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of EssayCompany.

There has been no common consensus among researchers regarding the best scoring and interpretation procedure for the verbal fluency task. This review provides a summary of various systems of qualitative scoring measures employed by investigators for deeper understanding of verbal fluency performance. Some of the qualitative measures include clustering and switching analysis, method of hierarchical exploration, error production analysis and time course analysis.

Clustering and switching analysis

One of the qualitative methods gaining wide popularity in verbal fluency outcome research is the understanding of mechanisms involved in the optimal word generation regarding characteristics and pattern of word generation. This endeavour helps in understanding not only regarding how well an examinee performs the task but also regarding how one goes about performing the task. The evidence from lesion studies and brain imaging has also provided evidence for the utility of this qualitative measurement in understanding the precise nature of deficient performance (Abwender et al., 2001; Troyer et al.,1997).

The earliest analysis of process of word generation has revealed that words generated during verbal fluency task occur in spurts with some meaningful relation between words rather than evenly in time. However, not all the words within the nested subset are recoverable, and individual tends to shift to next nested subset search in order to facilitate more number of word retrieval (Gruenewald & Lockhead, 1980; Wixted & Rohrer, 1994). Individuals to be successful in word retrieval needs to search for subcategories for the specified letter or category, retrieve the words from the subcategory and then move into next subcategory for retrieval once words have been exhausted.

In consonance with Chertkow and Bub's (1990) predictions, Troyer et al. (1997) proposed that the qualitative aspects of verbal fluency can be described based on two components, viz. Clustering and Switching. This was based on the emerging evidence of the multifactorial nature of verbal fluency task and due to the limitations of quantitative measure of total correct words of not being able to capture entirely all the important aspects of an individual's performance (Troyer et al., 1997). The terms clustering and switching was used by Troyer et al. (1997) to better operationalize the "store" and "search" processes involved in VF tasks, respectively. An effective and successful performance on verbal fluency requires an intact ability to produce words related semantically or phonemically (clustering) and an ability to shift efficiently to a new strategy once a category or subcategory is exhausted (switching). The output quantity is therefore determined by the semantic storage and the search component of verbal fluency.

The process of organizing words into semantically or phonemically related subcategories involves the production of clusters. Clusters produced during semantic task involve generation of two or more consecutive words that are related in meaning (e.g: apple, orange in fruits category; car, bus in vehicle category etc). In the same way, clusters produced during phonemic fluency task involves production of two or more consecutive words that are related based on phonemic characteristics (eg: words beginning with the same letter [chair, church]; words differing only by vowel [pan, pen] etc.). A task congruent clustering involving generation of semantically related words during semantic fluency task and phonemic based retrieval during phonemic fluency task has been reported (Raskin et al., 1992; Troyer et al., 1997). Utility of task-discrepant clustering (Abwender et al., 2000; Tallberg et al., 2011) which is a measure of intentional strategy use may also be involved wherin participants may retrieve phonemic clusters during semantic task or semantic clusters during phonemic fluency task. Another qualitative measure estimated during word generation task is the switching which involves calculation of the number of shifts between subcategories. The clusters produced are separated by gaps with the interval between words within clusters shorter than words between clusters (Gruenwald & Lockhead, 1980; Wixted & Roher, 1994). Troyer et al. (1997) postulated that clustering and switching are two dissociable components of verbal fluency with clustering and switching contributing equally to semantic fluency and switching contributing specifically for phonemic fluency. On animal fluency task, it is found that in terms of clustering, healthy adults produce clusters of two words per cluster and switched on an average 10.6 (3.5) times between the subcategories. Among older adults participants tend to switch less as compared to young adults, on average 8.5 (SD = 2.3) times between the subcategories with similar cluster size as younger adults (Troyer et al., 1997).

Each of the strategies used to maximize word production involves separate mechanisms involving specific brain areas. Performance on semantic memory processes of clustering (organizing words related to a subgroup) reflects the role of temporal lobe processes such as lexical or verbal memory storage. The executive processes of switching (engaging in strategic search processes) require mediation of frontal and frontal-subcortical area processes such as initiation, cognitive flexibility, and mental shifting. The evidence for these predictions is documented in clinical populations with predominant brain damage involving frontal or temporal lesions. Poor performance on clustering has been reported among patients with Alzheimer's disease (Troyer et al., 1998b), Mild Cognitive Impairment (Price et al., 2012), temporal lobe lesions (Henry & Crawford, 2004a; Troyer et al., 1998a) and temporal lobe epilepsy (Giovannetti et al., 2003).

Similarly, switching was reported to be impaired among patients with frontal lobe lesions (Troyer et al., 1998a), Parkinson's disease (Troster et al., 1998; Troyer et al., 1998b), Huntington's disease (Ho et al., 2002; Rich, Troyer, Bylsma, & Brandt, 1999), HIV associated dementia (Woods et al., 2004), multiple sclerosis (Henry & Beatty, 2006), depression (Henry & Crawford, 2005a), Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (Iudicello et al., 2007), schizophrenia (Robert et al., 1998) and under conditions of divided attention (Troyer et al., 1997). In conditions involving diffuse brain lesions, some investigators have also reported deficits in both clustering and switching with predominant influence on one. For instance, Troyer et al. (1998b) reported that in dementia of the Alzheimer's type, both clustering and switching is impaired, however the severity of impairment is noticed to be more on clustering than switching.

Recently there has been a shift in focus towards the development of clustering and switching during verbal fluency in children (Kave et al., 2008; Koren et al., 2005; Hurks et al., 2010; Sauzeon et al., 2004). Strategic processing during verbal fluency has been examined in clinical paediatric populations including children with PKU (Banerjee et al., 2011), Specific Language Impairment (Henry, Messer & Nash, 2012), blindness (Wakefield, Homewood, & Taylor, 2006), Turner syndrome (Temple, 2002) and Down syndrome (Nash & Snowling, 2008). As expected, both of these strategies are positively correlated with the total number of words produced (Kave et al., 2008; Koren et al., 2005; Robert et al., 1997; Troyer et al., 1997; Troster Fields, Testa et al., 1998).

Despite the clinical utility and good psychometric property, this qualitative measure is not without controversy. From the theoretical perspective, the most widely used Troyer protocol has been criticized by many researchers (Abwender et al., 2001; Demakis et al., 2003; Mayr, 2002; Ross et al., 2007). Abwender et al. (2000) criticized Troyer's protocol by stating that there is no adequate evidence that clustering inevitably leads to production of more words. They also state that the interpretation of switching whether it is a product of strategic searching and mental flexibility or lack of ability to cluster is not adequately explained. The consideration of single words as having cluster size of zero was also reported to not to take into consideration the failure to generate clusters. Epker, Lacritz, and Cullum (1999) observed that the qualitative measures didnot provide any additional information as compared to the total number of correct word calculation in differentiating individuals with AD, PD, and healthy older adults. Similarly Mayr (2002) criticized Troyer's scoring system regarding the ambiguous nature of switching score. They supported the view that it is difficult to differentiate whether the number of switches is associated with difficulties in accessing new clusters or difficulties in accessing new words within clusters. The more the time an individual spends in one cluster group; lesser the time will be for the individual to access other clusters. A reduction in the number of switches may be related to a general reduction in processing speed or selective switching deficit. Contrary to Troyer's view of clustering as an automatic process and switching as an effortful strategic process, Mayr and Kliegl (2000) reported involvement of the strategic component during both the processes. Demakis et al. 2003 considered switching component observed during verbal fluency performance as tapping general cognitive ability rather than specific executive processing. Koren et al. (2005) however considered number of clusters rather than number of switches as a measure of executive component. Similarly, Ross et al. (2007) questioned the consideration of clustering - switching as overt strategies of verbal fluency but rather an artefact of the test itself (especially clustering during letter fluency).

Reservations about diagnostic utility of clustering-switching measures have been raised by many researchers. From the clinical perspective, lack of consensus is found in terms of use of these measures in differentiating stable and declining individuals with Alzheimer's disease (Beatty et al., 2000), in individuals with moderate severity (Epker et al., 1999) with lack of differential sensitivity on clustering-switching in AD (Troyer et al., 1998b) and low test-retest reliability (Ross, 2003). The low temporal stability was related by Ross (2003) to lack of providing of explicit instruction to participants and therefore modification to the original instruction protocol was provided regarding the clustering strategies that may be employed during the procedure. Strauss, Sherman and Spreen (2006) however reported that qualitative analysis plays a significant role in individuals with focal lesions as well as mild cognitive dysfunction.

Hierarchical Exploration Analysis

Hierarchical exploration analysis as an outcome measure of semantic retrieval has been employed by many researchers (Beatty et al., 1989; Raboutet et al., 2010; Troster et al., 1995; Sauzeon et al., 2004). The task involved is word generation on supermarket fluency task (generating as many words as possible that can be purchased in a supermarket). The analysis involves a semantic categorical system comprising 10 categories with two hierarchical levels of items (category labels and category exemplars) for each category. The category label corresponds to super and subcategory nouns produced and category exemplars refer to the nouns of category specific items. For example in the category of fruits, the category labels include canned / frozen fruits and category exemplars include lemons, grapes, peaches etc. Based on the word output, a hierarchical ratio score (dividing the number of category labels (or exemplars) produced by the number of total words generated) is estimated. Troster et al. (1995) proposed scoring criteria for the classification of category labels and category exemplars.

Studies in clinical population have supported the use of hierarchical exploration analysis. For example, a selective decrease in the production of category exemplars has been shown in pathologies with temporal-lobe lesions, such as epilepsy or Alzheimer's disease (Martin & Fedio, 1983; N'Kaoua, Lespinet, Barsse, Rougier, & Claverie, 2001; Troster, Salmon, McCullough, & Butters, 1989; Troster et al., 1995). Similarly in children, Sauzeon et al. (2004) provided scoring for categories sampled, label and exemplar ratio, words per category sampled ratio and category shifts per category sampled ratio among French speaking children between 7-16 years of age.

Error Analysis

During the task of word generation for letter / category fluency, individuals tend to produce erroneous responses such as repeating the words (perseveration error) or coming up with words not starting with a particular letter or belonging to a particular category (rule break errors). From the cognitive perspective, the presence of errors is associated with a less effective control system and reduced executive capacities (Rosen & Engle, 1997) and as a strategic means employed by participants to generate new clusters (Troyer et al., 1997). While the Troyer et al protocol necessitates the inclusion of perseverations and errors in clustering-switching analysis, Haugrud, Lanting, and Crossley (2010) reported that their inclusion inflates the cluster size scores.

The error scores are often calculated as the number of individual error types present or a combined score for all the error types produced. Raboutet et al. (2010) calculated error score as the number of intrusions and perseverations produced. Robert and Le Dorze (1997) calculated the following five error types were scored: (1) repetition errors, repeating an item previously given as a correct response within the current test; (2) outside category errors, a word that did not belong to the category currently being tested; for example, saying ''veal'' in animals or ''pills'' in foods. (3) nonword or unintelligible errors; (4) wrong language errors, a word belonging to the appropriate category but not in the language currently being tested; and (5) other, any error not meeting one of the above definitions. Similarly, the error analysis has been part of many research studies (Hurks et al., 2004, 2006; Martin & Fedio, 1983; Martin et al., 1994; N'Kaoua et al., 2001; Raboutet et al., 2010; Raskin et al., 1992; Rosen & Engle, 1997; Troster et al., 1995).

Researchers have attempted to understand the error response pattern in disordered population in both adults and children which provides important information in clinical practice. Errors such as perseveration can be produced even in healthy controls, but these are mostly described in those individuals with neurological / cognitive deficits. The number of perseverations that adult subjects produce in their responses can have a diagnostic value (Troster et al., 1995). Compared to healthy adults, perseveration errors are frequent in individuals with Alzheimer's disease, aphasia, frontal lobe damage, Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease, and traumatic brain injury (Azuma, 2004). Pekkala, Albert, Spiro, and Erkinjuntti (2008) reported presence of recurring perseverations (fan, fried, friend, fan), continuous perseverations (fan, fan, fan) and stuck-in-set (continuing to name f words after a new letter has been presented) perseverations in Alzheimer's disease (AD).

Error analysis has gained popularity also in childhood verbal fluency research. The presence of more number of intrusion errors has been reported in children with ADHD (Mahone, Koth, Cutting, Singer, & Denckla, 2001). Similarly, in preschool children with early treated Phenylketonuria (Welsh, Pennington, Ozonoff, Rouse, & McCabe, 1990) more perseverative errors than control group were reported. Similarly error analysis has gained importance in typically developing children also (Charchat-Fichman et al., 2011; Hurks et al., 2004; Hurks et al., 2006; Tallberg et al., 2011). Tallberg et al. (2011) study in 130 typically developing children speaking Swedish language between 6 to 15 years of age reported presence of predominantly perseveration especially on letter fluency (FAS) as compared to the animal fluency task. Charchat-Fichman et al. (2011) study among Brazilian children (7-10 years) shown that the total number of errors during semantic fluency task correlated with phonemic fluency task though no significant correlation was noted with age with a relatively smaller number of errors.

Time course analysis

The organization of words during verbal fluency production is also examined as a function of time (Crowe, 1998; Hurks et al., 2010; Raboutet et al., 2010). Crowe (1996) proposed a model of lexical organization emphasizing the role of analyzing verbal fluency performance as a function of time, focussing on two types of store for retrieval of words. During the initial time frame (first 15-20 seconds) of verbal fluency task, participants generate words from a long term store called topicon which contains easily accessible common words. Once the topicon is exhausted, effortful search occurs in the store of extensive lexicon. Bousfield (1953) and Gruenewald and Lockhead (1980) showed that the time interval required to access new subcategories is long and increased during the production, whereas the time required to produce items within semantic clusters was short and tended to remain constant. Contrary to this, Mayr and Kliegl (2000) reported of equal contribution of executive and semantic component during word retrieval.

With respect of pattern of productivity over time, it is widely accepted that there exists a negative accelerated curve in terms of number of words produced over the time frame (Crowe, 1998; Hurks et al., 2004, 2006; Venegas & Mansur, 2011). It is observed that the number of words produced was greater with high frequency words produced with speed and accuracy during the first time interval than last intervals. This has been reported in all the participants (e.g., children, young adults, old adults; patients with schizophrenia, aphasia, depression, dementia) irrespective of the population studied (Crowe, 1998; Ober, Dronkers, Koss, Delis, & Friedland, 1986; Hurks et al., 2004, 2006; McDowd et al., 2011; Rosen, 1980). In children, studies (Hurks et al., 2006; Raboutet et al., 2010) have illustrated that the word frequency and number of words produced were observed to decrease with time with the greater score during 0-30 seconds as compared to 31-60 seconds. It was also noticed that the efficiency to avoid perseveration errors decreased with time, along with decrease in intercategorical process of hard switching. An increase in ratio of number of clusters produced, the number of category exemplars and mean cluster size. The varied performance across time was attributed to higher attention load and more effortful and extensive semantic search during the last time frame

Other Measures

Automated approaches using clustering algorithms to scoring consistent with Troyer and Mayr theories of verbal fluency have also been reported by many researchers. These techniques focuses on co-occurence frequencies and amount of competition between exemplars (items generated) for given category. Some of the computational methods include latent semantic analysis, correspondence analysis and hierarchical clustering and network theory (Goni et al., 2011; Schwartz, Baldo, Graves, & Brugger, 2003; Snyder & Munakata, 2010)

Standard Scoring Protocols

Various systems of scoring protocol for verbal fluency performance have been described in literature. The protocols vary in terms of the testing measures employed for analysis purposes. It is however to be kept in mind that a lot of disparity and disagreement exist between researchers on interpretation and utility of the protocol employed.

One of the most common and widely used protocols was given by Troyer et al. (1997). Troyer et al. focussed on analysis of number of words generated excluding errors and repetitions, clustering (number of clusters; cluster size; mean cluster size) and switching (number of switches). Cluster was operationally defined as production of successively generated words belonging to same subcategory (either phonemic or semantic subcategory). For the sequence cat-dog-lion-elephant-zebra, pet animals and African animals were considered as the two clusters produced by the individual. The cluster size, which is the number of words in a cluster, was counted beginning with the second word in each cluster (e.g., a 2-word cluster was counted as having a cluster size of 1). Single or nonclustered words were designated as having a cluster size of 0. The mean cluster size was calculated by adding up the size of each cluster and dividing by the number of clusters produced. For example, the sequence "lemons, chicken, meat, fruit, banana, apple, corn flakes, salt, pepper, cheese, milk, yogurt" contains 6 clusters, with respective cluster sizes of 0, 1, 2, 0, 1, 2 and a mean cluster size of 1.

With respect to definition of clusters, other researchers have attempted to refine Troyer and colleagues' protocol. Raskin et al. (1992) defined clusters as comprising of pair of words belonging to same subcategory without consideration for longer clusters or cluster size. Based on this, authors emphasized the role of number of clusters as a measure of cognitive flexibility rather than number of switches. Contrary to Troyer protocol, the ratio of total words to number of clusters was considered rather than the single word productions. Robert et al. (1998) considered three consecutive associated words in semantic fluency task and two consecutive associated words in phonemic fluency task as a cluster. In another protocol developed by Abwender et al. (2000) clusters was defined as two or more associated words. The authors did not consider single words as a cluster and hypothesized that single words suggest a failure to retrieve other words from that particular category. In Kosmidis et al. (2004a) scoring protocol, three or more consecutive words were grouped as a cluster for semantic and phonemic fluency.

With respect to switching, Troyer et al defined number of switches as transition between clusters including single words. Abwender et al. (2000) described two types of switches, that is, cluster switch and hard switch reflecting speeded nature of the task. Cluster switch involves transition from one cluster to next cluster and hard switch involves transition between two single words (banana, cheese) or between a cluster and a single word (fruit, banana, cheese). Abwender et al. provided an example for clustering-switching for word generation on food fluency. For the sequence of "banana, orange, milk, cheese", the number of clusters was considered as two (fruits; dairy products) with one cluster switch and no hard switch.

Other researchers have attempted to extend the protocol. March and Pattison (2006) along with Troyer system, provided scores for raw number of subcategories produced and number of errors (repetitions and categorical error types) in their study of individuals with AD. The number of semantic subcategories as an indicator of semantic memory organization was also reported by da Silva et al., (2004) in their study on impact of literacy and education in semantic fluency. Sauzeon et al. calculated ratio of total number of switches and mean cluster size to total number of words produced (reason for and against by Sauzeon and Troyer to be added in discussion). In Koren et al. (2005) study, instead of the number of switches, only number of clusters were analyzed. Similarly Tucha et al. (2005) alongside Troyer scoring system, counted the number of labels produced as well as clusters within clusters in individuals with ADHD. Lanting, Haugrud, and Crossley (2009) explored the number of novel and repeated clusters and percentage of clustered words in healthy and older adults. Kosmidis et al. (2004a) also provided a specific scoring protocol for animals, fruits and objects based on the data from Greek population.

Robert and Le Dorze (1997) reported use of scoring protocol consisting of total correct words (subcategory labels as in 'fruits' and category members as in 'apple'), number of errors (such as repetition errors, outside category errors such as ''pills'' in foods, nonword or unintelligible errors, wrong language errors, others). The analysis also involved analyzing of number of comments (such as swearing, self-talk (''that's all I know''), and questions about the task - ''can I say that?''). Scoring of number of semantic associations (three or more words belonging to same category), mean length of semantic association and percentage of words in semantic association (SA) were also reported as a part of scoring protocol by Roberts and Le Dorze (1997). In French/English balanced healthy bilinguals Roberts and Le Dorze (1997) reported similar semantic organization for animal and food fluency in both the languages. The mean number of SA's (4.47 in French; 4.84 in English), mean length of SA's (4.78 in French and 4.54 in English) and the mean percentage of words in SA's was 62.6 in French and 64.8 in English. Similarly Reverberi et al. (2006) used an index of semantic relatedness in their study on participants with frontal lesions.

Carneiro et al. (2008) in their study on Portugese category norms for children, reported the scoring of various measures including number of responses and exemplars, responses which are idiosyncratic and inappropriate and commonality and diversity indexes for the categories tested in children. Recently, Raboutet et al. (2010) provided a scoring system involving evaluation of five scores (general scores, intercategorical or switch scores, intracategorical or cluster scores, semantic hierarchical exploration scores and error scores) for each time interval involving supermarket fluency task.

Normal and abnormal aspects of verbal fluency - adults & children

The utility of verbal fluency has been researched in various populations including healthy individuals as well as disordered population. The research findings expanded below illustrates the extent of utility of verbal fluency tasks as a screening, diagnostic and treatment measure in various domains in both adult and childhood population.

Table 2.4

Summary of research findings on verbal fluency (VF) among adult population

Study population

Authors

Research findings

Healthy adults

Chan & Poon (1999); Loonstra, Tarlow, & Sellers (2001);Tombaugh et al., (1999); Troyer et al., (1997)

Influence of demographic variables evidenced with norm based data available in different languages and culture

Differential performance on phonemic and semantic based verbal fluency tasks

Peak performance in 19-30 years of age with subsequent decline

Older adults

Kemper & McDowd (2008); McDowd et al., (2011); Tombaugh et al., (1999); Troyer et al., (1997); Troyer (2000)

Aging related declines in verbal fluency (qualitative and quantitative measures)

Factors influencing verbal fluency performance documented (viz. age, level of education, gender, verbal intelligence, income, motor response, language effects, reading-writing speed, level of mental & physical activity, functional status)

Used as a test to differentiate aging from dementia due to superior performance

Focal cortical lesions (Frontal & Temporal lobe)

Antonucci, Beeson, Labiner, & Rapcsak (2008); Baldo et al., (2006); Baldo, Schwartz, Wilkins, & Dronkers (2010); Henry & Crawford (2004a); Stuss et al., (1998); Troyer et al., (1998a)

Frontal lobe lesions:

Frontal lobe involvement for letter fluency predominantly than category fluency

Non-conclusive findings on specific regions of frontal lobe involved

Selective deficits in switching

In non fluent aphasia, impaired phonemic clustering with preserved semantic clustering

Temporal lobe lesions:

Category fluency sensitive to temporal lobe damage depending on the extent of damage

Lesser deficits on phonemic fluency as compared to category fluency with selective deficits in clustering

Word generation greater on living than non living things

Impaired semantic clustering with preserved phonemic clustering in Wernicke aphasia

Alzheimer's disease (AD)

Butters et al., (1987); Cosentino, Scarmeas, Albert, & Stern (2006); Martin & Fedio (1983); McDowd et al., (2011); Monsch et al., (1992); Henry, Crawford, & Phillips (2004); Rohrer, Salmon, Wixted, & Paulsen (1999); Troyer et al., (1998b)

VF deficits seen early in the disease (impaired even in mild AD type) with rapid decline with time

Category fluency more affected than letter fluency

Deterioration in the structure, content and organization of semantic memory with fewer subordinate exemplars

Large proportion of response noted during the earlier part of recall time as a result of quick exhaustion of limited semantic store

No common consensus on whether the deficits reflect semantic store degradation or retrieval deficits related to executive control processes

Smaller clusters on both tasks and switched less often on semantic fluency than controls

Increased erroneous responses (perseverations and rule breaks)

VF task useful in diagnosis, predicting mortality, differential diagnosis (AD and elderly; cortical and subcortical dementia) & monitoring rate of decline

Huntington's disease (HD)

Henry, Crawford, & Phillips (2005); Ho et al., (2002); Monsch et al., (1992); Rich, Troyer, Bylsma, & Brandt, (1999); Rosser & Hodges (1994); Troster et al., (1989)

Decline in performance over time with disease progression

No common consensus on whether similar level of impairment for both tasks or greater deficits in LF than SF

Among the qualitative tasks, selective impairment in phonemic switching over time but not semantic switching with fewer semantic clusters

A larger proportion of recall noted during the last phase of recall time which results in a slower pattern of retrieval

Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB)

Ralph et al., (2001); Salmon et al., (1996)

Letter and category fluency equally reduced

Vascular dementia (VaD)

Carew et al., (1997); Jones, Laukka, & Backman (2006)

Both letter and category fluency impaired; Better performance than AD on SF task ;

HIV associated dementia

Woods et al., (2004)

Fewer words and switches with more intrusion errors

Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FLDT) subgroups

Kramer et al., (2003);Libon et al., (2007); Libon et al., (2009)

Differential pattern of impairment and neural activation reported across FLDT subtypes

VF employed as a task to differentiate between AD and FLDT subtypes

FLDT with behavioral/dysexecutive disorder:

Reduced performance on both tasks related to executive and semantic deficits

Letter fluency deficits related to bilateral frontal atrophy and semantic fluency deficits to left frontal/temporal atrophy

Semantic dementia:

Disproportionate impairment in SF related to the anterior and inferior left temporal lobe atrophy

Impaired lexical/semantic access more than mental search limitations

Progressive nonfluent aphasia:

Equally impaired on fluency tests resultant of Impaired lexical access

Deficits in semantic fluency to the right frontal lobe and letter fluency to left temporal atrophy

Progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP)

Rosser & Hodges (1994)

More impaired on letter fluency than semantic fluency

Deficits in initiation and retrieval mechanisms

Parkinson's disease (PD)

Donovan, Siegert, McDowall ,& Abernethy (1999); McDowd et al., (2011)

Troyer et al. (1998b)

Retrieval based on semantic & phonemic fluency impaired

Less switching ability with no difference on cluster size as compared to control group

Dementia with Parkinson's disease (DPD):

Switched less often on both tasks and produced smaller clusters on phonemic fluency than controls

Measures of clustering and switching discriminate the dementia groups from their respective control groups (DPD vs AD)

Nondemented patients with Parkinson's disease (NPD):

Not impaired on any fluency variable (TNCW/qualitative) measures

Decreased performance in semantic and alternating word fluency task

Schizophrenia

Allen, Liddle, & Frith (1993); Bokat & Goldberg (2003); Bozikas, Kosmidis, & Karavatos (2005); Giovannetti et al., (2003); Joyce, Collinson, & Crichton (1996); Koychev et al., 2011; Henry & Crawford (2005b)

Disproportionate impairment in semantic fluency

Selective semantic memory impairment ("a poorly organized search through a large word pool") with defective strategy use

Significant improvement with cueing and worsening with course of illness

Lack of consensus on whether the nature of semantic deficits (access or storage)

Intact clustering and impaired switching between recall strategies

VF as a cognitive biomarker to detect schizotypy phenotype and its reversal by psychotropic drugs

Semantic fluency as the most severely impaired test in schizophrenia relative to other neuropsychological tests

Useful as a possible predictor of psychosis

Variation in scores depending on presence of associated symptoms

Epilepsy

Drane et al., (2006); Giovannetti et al., (2003)

Differences in performance on semantic fluency based on the presence of frontal or temporal lobe seizure onset (greater performance improvement in frontal lobe seizure than temporal lobe onset on cued semantic fluency task)

Temporal lobe epilepsy: poor with regards to fluency output but showed impaired clustering with intact switching

Cerebellar diseases

Appollonio et al., (1993); Burk et al., (1999); Leggio et al., (2000); Silveri & Misciagna, (2000)

Impairment on VF tasks (greater effect on semantic fluency)

TBI/ Head Injury

Borkowski et al., (1967); Crawford, Moore, & Cameron (1992); Henry & Crawford (2004b); Kave, Heled, Vakil, & Agranoc (2011); Raskin & Rearick (1996)

VF tests sensitive to TBI, even in mild TBI cases with significant effects of severity

Utility of executive component of SF to assess executive deficits after TBI

Fewer words on LF increasing with increased letter difficulty depending on verbal IQ level

Differences in total output, number of switches and clusters but not mean cluster size as compared to control group

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI)

Murphy, Rich, & Troyer (2006); Price et al., (2012)

Significant impairment in category fluency (reduced word output, smaller cluster sizes, accessed fewer subcategories, switching frequency fairly better)

Difficulties in isolating semantic categories and loss of associative links within semantic categories

Depression

Fossati et al., (2003); Henry & Crawford(2005a); Lafont et al., (1998)

Verbal fluency tasks sensitivity to depression noted

Fewer words,  reduced number of switches with normal cluster sizes

Useful in differential diagnosis from early stages of AD

Related to generalized impairment of cognitive slowing

Bipolar disorders

Allin et al., (2010) ; Costafreda et al., (2011)

Distinct neural responses to VF task as compared to schizophrenia

Increased error production and

Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS)

Iudicello et al., (2007); Iudicello, Woods, Deutsch, & Grant, (2012); Milikin, Trepanier, & Rourke (2004)

Lower verbal fluency performance

Impaired switching but not clustering

Effects predominant in older adults with HIV

Right Hemisphere Damaged

Joanette & Goulet, (1986); Mayer (2005); Varley (1995)

Word retrieval on VF task affected

Fewer responses and lexical retrieval strategies

Intact lexical semantic knowledge with failure to use lexical knowledge flexibility

Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis

Abraham et al.,( 2000); Abrahams et al., (2004); Lepow et al., (2010)

VF impairment as a result of higher order cognitive deficits

Fewer clusters and more switches with differences dependant on cognitive involvement

Multiple Sclerosis

Friend et al., (1999); Henry & Beatty (2006); Vlaar & Wade (2003)

VF as a sensitive neuropsychological measures to cognitive impairment in MS

VF impairment for LF and SF tasks

Klinefelter Syndrome

Boone et al., (2001); Patwardhan et al., (2000)

Reduced retrieval abilities on VF

Employed as a task of executive dysfunction testing

Adults with ASD

Lopez, Lincoln, Ozonoff, & Lai (2005); Spek, Schatorje, Scholte, & Berckelaer-Onnes (2009)

Lower scores

Improvement in scores with increase in age

Bilingual population

Bialystok, Craik, & Luk,(2008); Gollan, Montoya, & Werner (2002); Luo, Luk, & Bialystok (2010); Roberts & Le Dorze (1998); Roberts & Le Dorze (1997); Rosselli, et al., (2000)

Bilingual Aphasia:

No consistent between language difference in productivity and semantic organization on semantic fluency

Neurologically unimpaired population:

Reduced VF performance compared to Monolingual, predominantly on semantic as compared to letter fluency

Slower and effortful word retrieval

Differences related to smaller overall vocabulary size and slower word retrieval related to competition resolution with the other language in bilinguals especially for category fluency

Similar number of exemplars in balanced bilinguals indicating similar performance in both languages (similar semantic organization)

Verbal fluency in adult population

The Table 4 given below illustrates the summary of the studies carried out in adult population including the various clinical populations. As shown, the clinical and research utility of verbal fluency is well researched and well established and continues to be one of the important assessment tool for assessing language and neuropsychological functioning.

Table 4: Summary of research findings on verbal fluency (VF) among adult population

Verbal fluency in typically developing children

The earliest norms on verbal fluency in healthy school children had been provided for phonemic fluency (FAS) task by Gaddes & Crockett (1975); Sincoff & Sternberg (1988) in 135 children belonging to grades III and VI task; Delis et al. (2001) as cited in Mitrushina et al. 2005; Anderson et al. (1997) as cited in Spreen et al. (2006) in 390 children between the age range of 7-13 years from primary and secondary schools in Melbourne, Australia and Anderson et al. (2001) in 32 adolescents between ages of 14-15 years. Similarly, Schum et al. (1989) as cited in Spreen et al. (2006) provided data on CFL and PRW phoneme fluency task across both males and females (98:131) between 6-12 (n=229) years of age and Barr (2003) investigated verbal fluency using CFL version among 100 high school athlete children (60 males and 40 females) between 13-17 years. Halperin et al. (1989) investigated performance in 204 children between 6-12 years of age on phoneme fluency ('sh'). Cohen, Morgan, Vaughn, Riccio, and Hall (1999) studied the maximum number of words retrieved by typically developing children on verbal fluency. The letter fluency tasks (letters C, P, B, R) in a thirty second time frame for each letter was used for normal children aged 6.0 to 12.11 years divided into seven age bands. Statistically significant difference was noted between 6 years and 8-12 years, however, 6 years were not significantly different from seven year old children. This pattern of nearer age bands not showing significant differences was noted even in higher age groups.

Performance on semantic fluency had also been reported by Nelson (1974) on nine natural language categories in five and eight year old children; Posnansky (1978) on 25 categories in children between grades 2 and 6; Storm (1980) in children (6 years, 9 years, teenagers) on animal fluency task; Halperin et al. (1989) on animal and food fluency; Lucariello et al. (1992) on tasks of clothes, animals, food, furniture and tools among four and seven year old children; Grube & Hasselhorn (1996) in eight-year-olds and older children on animal fluency and Crowe and Prescott (2003) for the categories of animal, body parts, food, clothes, vehicles and plant in 155 children between 5-10 years of age.

Studies on verbal fluency had also been carried among healthy children in various languages other than English including Chinese (Chan & Poon in 1999 provided data on category fluency of animal and transportation between 7-95 years), Cantonese-speaking Hongkong Chinese adolescents (Lee et al., 2002, as cited in Mitrushina et al., 2005 on animal naming and fruits and vegetables), Italian (Riva, Nichelli & Devoti, 2000), French (Sauzeon et al., 2004), Spanish speaking children (Ardila & Rosselli, 1994; Matute, Rosselli, Ardila and Morales, 2004; Nieto, 2008; Filippetti & Allegri, 2011), Brazilian population (Dellatolas et al., 2003; Charchat-Fichman et al., 2011), Portugese (Carneiro et al., 2008), Dutch (Hurks et al., 2006; Hurks et al., 2010; Hurks et al., 2012; Van der Elst et al., 2011), Hebrew language (Koren et al., 2005; Kave, 2006; Kave et al., 2008; Kave et al., 2010) and Swedish language (FAS and animal fluency: Tallberg, Carlsson & Lieberman, 2011). Similar studies have been carried out among Spanish-English bilingual children (Rosselli, Ardila, Navarrete & Matute, 2010) where they have reported of reduced performance on verbal fluency tasks with a predominant bilingual disadvantage for semantic fluency task in bilingual speakers as compared to monolingual speakers.

An increase in the number of research carried out in typically developing children was noted from early 2000 onwards using variations in type of tasks, procedure and analysis protocol employed in numerous languages.

Developmental changes in semantic and phonemic fluency in 153 Italian school children between 5 years 11months to 11 years 4 months (classified into five groups depending on the grade) was reported by Riva et al. (2000). Total number of words retrieved on the phonemic fluency tasks (B and S) and semantic fluency (animals and food) were calculated. Significant age related differences were noticed only on semantic fluency task with phonemic fluency tasks considerably difficult compared to semantic fluency tasks. The difficulty on phonemic fluency task was attributed to the task dependency on frontal lobe maturation and requirement of greater organizational and strategic capabilities. Linear increase in fluency scores especially in semantic fluency tasks was noted with an increase in age with a significant increase between Group I and Group II. The findings were related to cognitive development, formal instructions and enrichment in linguistic knowledge between the ages of 5 and 7 years.

Sauzeon et al. 2004 studied five groups of children(n=140) between the age range of 7-16 years (second grade to tenth grade) on their performance on verbal fluency of three letter fluency tasks (F, A and S), two semantic fluency tasks (fruit and supermarket fluency), two sound fluency tasks (/ma/,/o/) and one free fluency tasks. The influence of age on verbal fluency was assessed in French in terms of total number of words, clustering and switching and semantic network exploration. Findings revealed a greater difference between 7-8- and 9-10- year old children which gradually decreased with age. These differences were noted more on semantic fluency as compared to letter fluency tasks which showed improvement with age attributing to dependency on late developing cognitive strategies and late brain maturation. Similarly, greater changes with age in switches and cluster measure except for cluster size were evident in the letter fluency task. Proficient semantic network exploration was reported only after 13-14 years of age. The authors concluded that semantic knowledge enrichment is associated with semantic fluency while, strategic switching is associated with letter fluency which could be used as an early indicator for strategic or semantic deficits in childhood developmental disorders.

Matute et al. (2004) studied verbal fluency among Spanish speaking children attending public and private schools in Guadalajara and Tijuana, Mexico. 171 children (81 boys, 90 girls) between ages 6 and 15 years performed semantic (fruits) and phonologic fluency (letter M). The study findings revealed significant age effect and authors supported the need for norm based cross-language studies. Nieto (2008) provided normative data for Spanish speaking children on Phonemic (letters FAM) and semantic (animals) fluency tasks. Verbal fluency performance (number of words, number of clusters, switches and mean cluster size) in 79 school aged children were investigated. It was found that there were developmental changes in terms of number of words, clusters and switches generated with older group performing better than younger age group. On phonemic fluency task, there was a significant difference noted between older children and 6-7 year old children, whereas on semantic fluency the significant difference was noted only between 10-11 years and younger children. These developmental differences were related to frontal lobe maturation and development of cognitive flexibility.

Koren et al. (2005) conducted a study comparing third (8-9 years) and fifth grade (10-11 years) children on development of clustering abilities (number of clusters and mean cluster size) on 5 phonological fluency tasks (g, d, p, r, sh) and 4 semantic fluency tasks ('animals', 'food', 'clothes' and 'streets') in Hebrew. The authors reported an increased number of clusters in the higher age group; though mean cluster size did not show any age effect which had been attributed to the development of cognitive flexibility and change in organization of output with age.

The development of verbal fluency as a function of age has been studied in one hundred and fifty Hebrew-speaking children by Kave (2006) between the age range of 8 and 17 years on three phonemic fluency (bet /b/, gimel /g/,and shin /Å¡/) and three semantic fluency (animals, fruits and vegetables, vehicles) tasks. A steady increase in number of words with age was reported with 16-17 year old adolescents reaching adult level only on phonemic fluency task. The findings were attributed to vocabulary enrichment and due to the improvement in ability to retrieve stored vocabulary efficiently with age. In a later study in 2008, Kave et al. compared clustering and switching measures on phonemic and semantic task across 8-29 years. Improvement in verbal fluency measures was reported to increase in age except for mean cluster size. Comparison of adolescents and adults revealed no significant difference in phonemic fluency task, however on semantic fluency task, the adults were found to perform better than adolescents on switching scores. Significant correlations were noted between total number of words and clustering measures (number of clusters, mean cluster size) and switching measures (number of switches) on both tasks except no significant correlation between the total number of words and mean cluster size. The authors attributed study results more towards development in executive search strategies and shifting than maturation in word retrieval. Recently in 2010, Kave et al. studied phonemic and semantic fluency in 207 Hebrew speaking children between 8-17 years. This study also provided evidence of a positive correlation between age and test scores.

Hurks et al. (2006) compared verbal fluency performance (semantic category versus initial letter fluency) in 91 healthy school going Dutch speaking children (8.4-9.7 years) from Netherlands using an alternative scoring protocol of verbal fluency focussing on the word production as a function of time. The two aspects taken into consideration for verbal fluency scoring was automatic information processing (word production during first 15 seconds of verbal fluency task) and controlled information processing (word production during the next 45 seconds). It was observed that the word production was greater with less error production during semantic category fluency as compared to initial letter fluency. As a function of time, a decrease in performance was noted during controlled processing. Hurks et al. (2010) provided a detailed investigation report of developmental changes in semantic verbal fluency among 309 native Dutch children attending first to ninth grade. The authors reported that the controlled information processing was observed to be established by grades 7-8 only. With respect to demographic variable of gender, no positive influence was noted in both the studies. However the performance was found to be influenced by the level of occupational achievement of caregiver and parental education. Recently, Hurks (2012) did a comparison study on effects of brief training on strategy use during verbal fluency task in 81 children between grades 3-6. The influence of instruction was observed only in older children, whereas the younger population continue to use cognitive capacity to increase the semantic clustering scores. In 294 healthy Dutch speaking children between 6.56 - 15.85 years, Van der Elst et al. (2011) also studied animal verbal fluency performance and provided the norm based findings. The research outcome indicated a linear pattern in productivity (that is, total number of correct unrepeated animals) as a function of age with positive influence of parental education and lack of gender effect.

Carneiro et al. (2009) investigated 300 Portugese speaking children belonging to preschoolers between 3-4 years (M:F-54:46), second grade between 7-8 years (M:F-49:51) and preadolescents (M:F-49:51) between 11-12 years. The category fluency was tested using the 13 categories in preschool group, 17 categories in the second grade group and 21 categories in preadolescents group. The authors provided norms with findings regarding the frequency of sub category production, number of responses, inappropriate and idiosyncratic responses and commonality (calculated by adding the frequency of the three first exemplars and then dividing by the total frequency production) and diversity indexes (the degree to which the exemplars are distinct).

Recently, Charchat-Fichman et al. (2011) studied phonemic and semantic fluency in one hundred and nineteen Brazilian children between the age range of 7-10 years (male: female- 59:60). Phonemic fluency was tested using F, A, M letters and semantic fluency using animals, clothes and fruits category. The total number of correct words produced during each task was computed along with rate of error production. Total number of words generated was greater for semantic fluency task as compared to phonemic fluency tasks. The authors related the findings to greater requirements of executive function and strategic retrieval in phonemic fluency and semantic network activation during semantic fluency task. Significant difference was noted among the three semantic tasks but not during phonemic fluency task. Though no effect of gender was noted, significant age difference was noted with lesser number of words in younger age group than older children. Total number of errors during semantic fluency task correlated with phonemic fluency task though no significant correlation was noted with age. The authors reported that the number of errors is very small for the age range taken for the study. Similarly, Dellatolas et al. (2003) studied semantic fluency (animals, clothes) and phonemic fluency (letters P, F, M) in 41 school children (7-8 years) from Brazilian population. On comparison of the performance with 97 illiterate normal Brazilian adults, lower performance was reported for semantic fluency in school children.

Tallberg et al. (2011) studied strategies employed by 130 children speaking Swedish language between 6 to 15 years of age. FAS and animal fluency task was used to analyze the mean scores, clustering, switching and error production in these children. Effective switching and clustering strategies were reported in higher age children along with better mean scores for both the tasks. The authors suggested that VF continues to develop into early adulthood. It was also reported that gender does not have an influence on VF performance. The detailed error production analysis revealed predominantly perseveration especially on letter fluency task. The study also provided support for earlier works regarding semantic facilitation during not only semantic fluency but also during the phonological fluency task.

Based on the verbal fluency study among Spanish speaking children between 8-11 years (n=120), Filippetti and Allegri (2011) reported the role of the task in measuring executive functioning in children. In consonance with previous literature, greater verbal output (total score) was noted for semantic as compared to phonological fluency especially in older children with a decrease in scores over time. The authors also reported correlations between qualitative strategies (clustering; switching) and total score as well as with cognitive executive functions.

On the verbal fluency subtest of NEPSY, a score of 8.48 (3.12) in 9-year-olds (n=25) and 8.55 (2.74) in 11-year olds (n=20) were reported in Zambian school children (Mulenga, Ahonen, & Aro, 2001). The study compared the performance of 45 literate Zambian children according to age-equivalent norms for U.S. children. Similarly, Korkman, Kemp, and Kirk (2001) reported significant age effects especially for 5-8 year age range than 9-12 year age band in a study on 800 children from the United States between 5-12 years of age.



rev

Our Service Portfolio

jb

Want To Place An Order Quickly?

Then shoot us a message on Whatsapp, WeChat or Gmail. We are available 24/7 to assist you.

whatsapp

Do not panic, you are at the right place

jb

Visit Our essay writting help page to get all the details and guidence on availing our assiatance service.

Get 20% Discount, Now
£19 £14/ Per Page
14 days delivery time

Our writting assistance service is undoubtedly one of the most affordable writting assistance services and we have highly qualified professionls to help you with your work. So what are you waiting for, click below to order now.

Get An Instant Quote

ORDER TODAY!

Our experts are ready to assist you, call us to get a free quote or order now to get succeed in your academics writing.

Get a Free Quote Order Now