ADHD: Context, Effects and Models of Intervention

Print   

25 Jan 2018

Disclaimer:
This essay has been written and submitted by students and is not an example of our work. Please click this link to view samples of our professional work witten by our professional essay writers. Any opinions, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of EssayCompany.

Examining Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Introduction

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder has been recently acknowledged as a condition that affects the whole life experience of children with this disorder. The condition is chronic, no actual cures seem imminent. However, effective treatment interventions exist currently and more effective treatments are under development.

The following is an examination of ADHD from several perspectives. The historical concepts, issues and viewpoints regarding this disorder prove a rich ground for exploring advances, retrenchments and side roads along the timeline.

More in depth of discussion of diagnosis and treatment models for ADHD is provided. The complications introduced by the fact of comorbid conditions are discussed at some length. The details presented on models of treatment include portions of a medical and portions of a social model, well-suited to the coordination skills of a social worker to play a pivotal role.

The various impacts of ADHD, especially on the child, are discussed, together with implications for relieving these burdens.

In conclusion, a brief discussion of the social workers role in the ADHD constellation of requirements and opportunities is presented.

Historical Concepts, Issues and Perspectives

Largely due to changes in naming conventions, shifts in conceptualization and the spate of public and professional controversy, ADHD is perceived by many as a recently-identified disorder. In fact, however, the history of the concept is quite long, with a timeline stretching back to the mid-19th Century.

ADHD may have been first described by Dr. Heinrich Hoffman in 1845. A German physician who wrote on medicine and psychiatry, Dr. Hoffman also authored poetry for children. His verse concerning a fidgety young man named Philip made its way into the British medical journal Lancet in 1904. This is often cited as the first account of ADHD published in the medical literature:

The Story of Fidgety Philip

"Let me see if Philip can

Be a little gentleman;

Let me see if he is able

To sit still for once at the table."

Thus Papa bade Phil behave;

And Mama looked very grave.

But Fidgety Phil,

He won't sit still;

He wriggles,

And giggles,

And then, I declare,

Swings backwards and forwards,

And tilts up his chair,

Just like any rocking horse—

"Philip! I am getting cross!"

See the naughty, restless child

Growing still more rude and wild,

Till his chair falls over quite.

Philip screams with all his might,

Catches at the cloth, but then

That makes matters worse again.

Down upon the ground they fall,

Glasses, plates, knives, forks and all.

How Mama did fret and frown,

When she saw them tumbling down!

And Papa made such a face!

Philip is in sad disgrace . . . (Hallowell & Ratey, 1995, pp. 270-271).

Two years before the publication of this issue of Lancet, the pediatrician Sir George F. Still delivered a series of three lectures to England’s Royal Academy of Physicians, describing a group of twenty children whose behavior he characterized as impulsive, manifesting significant behavior problems. Given that children judged to be subject to poor child-rearing were excluded from these observations, Still concluded that the cause of these unbounded behaviors was likely genetic or a result of brain damage (Still, 1902).

Still’s proposition constituted a novel approach. Previously, objectionable behavior was assumed to be the product of voluntary choice on the part of the child and/or poor parenting, and physical punishment was the ‘treatment’ of choice. The offer of genetic and neurological dynamics as potential controlling factors ushered in a “kinder, more effective approach to child-rearing” (Hallowell & Ratey, 1995, p. 272).

As a result of the 1918 influenza pandemic, Still’s attribution of ADHD-like constellations of behavior to brain damage was reinforced by behavior patterns of many children recovering from encephalitis. These children were reported to exhibit “restlessness, inattention, impulsivity, easy arousability, and hyperactivity” (Wolraich, 2006, p. 86). Several publications, notably Kahn and Cohen’s 1934 article in the New England Journal of Medicine (Hallowell & Ratey, 1995, p. 272), provided the link between this neurological condition and the symptoms of distractibility, impulsivity and restlessness, now considered diagnostic of ADHD.

A few years later, in 1937, Dr. Charles Bradley demonstrated the apparently paradoxical effect of the stimulant Benzedrine in controlling hyperactivity in children. The finding that behavior problems improved with stimulant medication reinforced the notion of a biological basis. This link received convincing empirical support. In 1957, a technological boost arrived with the release of the stimulant methylphenidate (Ritalin; Hallowell & Ratey, 1995, p. 272; Wolraich, 2006, p. 87-88). Additional advances occurred in 1999, with the advent of extended-release stimulants and in 2003, with the approval of Atomoxetine (Strattera), a nonstimulant medication, for the treatment of ADHD in children and adults (see Troost et al., 2006). Coghill (2005) pointed out that “extended-release stimulant preparations and novel non-stimulant treatments for ADHD potentially offer real benefits for patient care” (p. 290).

When Ritalin became available in 1957, the disorder to be treated was termed Minimal Brain Damage. In 1966, the recognition that clear evidence of brain damage was absent in many cases prompted re-labeling the problem as Minimal Brain Dysfunction. The following year, DSM-II re-christened this diagnosis as the Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood Disorder, reflecting growing doubt regarding brain damage involvement. Also, research in the 1960s, including that of Stella Chase, placed an emphasis on hyperactivity as the major component of this disorder (Wolraich, 2006, p. 86-87).

Another potential cause of the disorder was introduced by the work of Dr. Ben Feingold in 1973. His argument that instances of hyperactivity were increasing as the levels of food additives rose promoted an alternative treatment literature based on diet and nutrition. This step along the ADHD timeline also reinforced the role of biology in the disorder through the mechanism of allergic reactions (Eigenmann & Haenggeli, 2004; Feingold, 1973).

The diagnostic labeled changed again with DSM-III in 1980. Reflecting a conceptual shift from hyperactivity to inattention as the focal deficit, the term Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) replaced Hyperkinetic Reaction and two subtypes were defined: ADD with and without hyperactivity. Seven years later, DSM-III-R provided the current label of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), while scrapping the subtypes due to lack of evidence. DSM-IV retained the label ADHD and redefined three subtypes: predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive, and combined (Wolraich, 2006, p. 87). Wolraich (2006) speculated that recent research emphasizing impulse control, such as that reported by Barkley in 1997, may spur yet another reconceptualization of this diagnosis.

While the core diagnostic symptoms of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity have persevered across the timeline, the specific diagnostic criteria have changed to follow the focus of shifting conceptualizations of the disorder. Not only can shifts in concepts and labels be confusing, the concommitant adjustment of these criteria can alter incidence and prevalence statistics. For example, within the same sample, Wolraich (2006) cited an increase in ADHD prevalence rates from 2.6 % with DSM-III to 6.1% using DSM-III-R (p. 87).

All along the timeline, differences in concept and terminology flourish. These difference also include those that are cross-cultural in nature and those inspired by comorbidity. For example, the label DAMP (Deficits in Attention, Motor Control and Perception) is used only in Sweden and Denmark to designate ADHD co-occurring with dyspraxia (Gillberg, 2003).

The International Classification of Diseases (ICD) uses the label of HKD (Hyperkinetic Disorder) for symptomatology similar to ADHD in the DSM; for global discourse, the two are used interchangeably, although ICD-10’s stricter standards yield lower incidence rates (Remschmidt, 2005). Other examples abound.

Hallowell and Ratey (1995) characterized this journey through ADHD history as “riddled with false leads, multiple possibilities, contradictory findings, and many gut reactions of all kinds” (p. 274). It is no wonder that a fair number of frustrated stakeholders assert that ADHD is a mythic disorder, a social construct, without biological or psychological validity, perpetrated on society by profit-seeking drug companies and service providers (e.g., Neufeld & Foy, 2006; Timini & Taylor, 2004). Responses to these doubts regarding the validity and impact of ADHD constitute the final three points on this timeline.

Validity concerns, especially those related to low sample size, were addressed in 1999 by the multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD (MTA Study). Five hundred seventy-nine with ADHD, aged 7 to 10 years, were randomly assigned to four 14-month treatment conditions at six sites in the United States and Canada. The initial findings from this study indicated that medication alone was more effective than was only psychosocial treatment, and that the combination of treatments benefited some children more than medication only condition (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999). This substantial dataset has been used in the publication of more than forty studies.

In 2002, the world’s leading experts on ADHD joined together to publish the International Consensus Statement on ADHD, a strongly-worded document intended to put to rest the arguments against the validity and impact of this disorder, especially those propagated in the media (Barkley et al., 2002). This statement set out a two-part litmus test for a valid medical or psychiatric disorder: “…there must be scientifically established evidence that those suffering from the condition have a serious deficiency in or failure of a physical or psychological mechanism that is universal to humans…regardless of culture…and…that this serious deficiency leads to harm to the individual” (p. 89). Striving to meet, and exceed, both criteria, the statement relied on reference to research demonstrating deficits in specific neurological functions; cross-cultural twin studies, demonstrating the primary role of genetic contribution; and the abundance of data bearing on the lasting negative impact of ADHD for major life activities of the individual, as well as for the family, school, community and society (p. 90).

Following a similar mission, the Global ADHD Working Group set out to foster consensus on the validity and impact of this disorder, and to reach a consensus on best practice in diagnosis and treatment. The Group consisted of fifteen recognized experts from Australia, Brazil, France, Germany, South Korea, Mexico, Philippines, UK and USA. The outcome of their work was published as the Global Consensus on ADHD/HKD (Remschmidt, 2005). In addition to fleshing out the International Consensus Statement’s arguments for validity and impact, this Group addressed the importance of accurate diagnosis and measurement of impairment, highlighting the similarities and differences between ADHD (in DSM-IV) and HKD (in ICD-10). For example, the Group noted that a re-analysis of the MTA Study data to investigate the implications of using the ICD-10 classification system indicated that 25% of the sample diagnosed as ADHD would have received the HKD diagnosis under ICD-10 (p. 129).

The bulk of this Group’s work focused on achieving consensus treatment algorithms, designed to be used at a global level. These four algorithms are guidelines intended to aid clinical judgment in the multimodal treatment of ADHD alone, and ADHD with comorbidities present, including conduct disorder, major depressive disorder, and tic disorder (pp. 130-136). This highlights the pivotal role of comorbidity in conditioning the processes of ADHD diagnosis and treatment.

The next section examines the evidence and implications of some comorbidity findings, as well as other aspects of the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD.

Some Issues in Diagnosis and Treatment

Co-occurring Disorders

ADHD is associated with a high co-occurrence of other psychiatric disorders. More often than not, ADHD is accompanied by a learning disability. Each comorbid disorder serves to modify the clinical presentation of ADHD, challenging the diagnostic and treatment planning process.

For example, a review study by James et al. (2004) supported a connection between ADHD and suicide. Compared with USA national suicide rates and risk ratios for males between ages of five and twenty-four years, ADHD appears to increase this risk ratio for males via increasing the severity of comorbid conditions, especially conduct disorder (CD) and depression.

Common disorders that frequently co-exist with ADHD include:

  • Oppositional Defiant Disorder (and Conduct Disorder)
  • Learning and communication differences
  • Anxiety (state or trait)
  • Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
  • Depression
  • Enuresis
  • Drug abuse
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Sleep Problems
  • Tourettes Disorder
  • Pervasive Developmental Disorder
  • Many forms of physical illness (such as asthma)
  • Accidental injury

Faraone (2001) reviewed these patterns of comorbidity. The high instance of comorbidity has given rise to controversy as to whether ADHD exists as a primary disorder or only secondary to other psychiatric syndromes. The nosological system advocated in the DSM is a hierarchical one; that is, in the presence of two or more diagnoses, one is to be considered primary and should account for significant symptoms observed in the secondary disorder. There is mounting evidence, however, that many conditions exist concurrently with ADHD, and each modifies the overall clinical presentation and treatment response. Faraone argued that comorbid conditions should be considered simultaneously in order to broaden our understanding and maximize treatment.

Depressed patients may demonstrate diminished concentration and individuals with bipolar disorder often manifest psychomotor ‘hyperactivity’ and distractibility. It may be difficult to differentiate these symptoms from the core symptoms of ADHD. There are several ways to deal with symptom overlap in research and clinical settings. The "subtraction method" requires the same absolute number of symptoms to be present to diagnose either disorder, except that shared symptoms are "subtracted" or removed from diagnostic consideration. For example, 8 of 14 (57%) symptoms are needed for the diagnosis of ADHD in the DSM-IV. If two overlap symptoms are present between the two co-occurring disorders, then 8 of 12 (67%) symptoms would be required to make the diagnosis with the subtraction method. Alternatively, in the "proportional" method the same ratio of symptoms would be required. Milberger et al. (1995) reported that most patients with comorbid depression and ADHD still retained the diagnosis of ADHD whether the subtraction or proportional method was used. Long-term follow-up studies have demonstrated that individuals with ADHD and comorbid disorders have poorer prognoses and higher rates of hospitalization than those with ADHD alone (Biederman et al., 1998).

Pharmacologic "dissection" studies have been conducted to determine clinical contributions of each of the comorbid disorders and treatment strategies. For example, an individual with comorbid ADHD and bipolar disorder may first be treated with stimulants. Usually, little change occurs with this treatment intervention. Alternatively, if a mood stabilizer is initiated first, significant improvement may be expected. If symptoms of ADHD continue to persist after mood stabilization, this gives support for the diagnosis of two co-occurring diagnoses. The addition of a stimulant may then be indicated. Therefore, unless both comorbid conditions are adequately treated, the ADHD symptoms may not remit. Pharmacologic studies of bipolar disorder and ADHD suggest that the two disorders exist as discrete entities in many patients (Biederman et al., 1998).

ADHD with Asperger Syndrome. Asperger Syndrome (AS) is a pervasive developmental disorder characterized by deficits in social interaction and motor coordination, as well as by unusual or restricted patterns of interest and behavior. The clinical distinction between autism and AS has to do with severity and qualitative expression of the diagnostic criteria. Both are characterized by deficits in social interaction, impairment of communication skills, and the manifestation of unusual or bizarre behaviors. However, in AS, motor deficits are more pronounced, onset occurs later, and social deficits are present without grossly impaired speech and language (Frith, 1991).

There are relatively few studies of psychiatric disorders comorbid with AS. It is reported that 28% of AS subjects have co-occurring ADHD. The comorbidity of these two disorders vary according to developmental level; ADHD appears to be more common in the younger AS population (Klin & Volkmar, 1997).

ADHD with depressive disorders. The overlap between depression and ADHD is now well recognized. Given that the presence of an underlying or co-occurring mood disorder complicates the treatment of ADHD, proper recognition and treatment of this disorder is vital.

Recent evidence indicates that depressive disorders may be manifested in childhood in a variety of guises. Depressed children tend to present with irritability, negativism, social withdrawal, school dysfunction, and somatic disorders. AS with the core symptoms of ADHD, these symptoms can be attributed to normal childhood behaviors. Hence, the diagnosis may be overlooked.

The rate of comorbidity between ADHD and depression was examined in a long-term follow-up study by Biederman and colleagues (1996). The baseline rate of major depression in children diagnosed with ADHD was approximately 30%. At four years, the rate was over 40% in children with ADHD compared with approximately 5% in controls. The children with comorbid disorders manifested high rates of a variety of disorders including bipolar disorder and anxiety disorder. The comorbid group was more dysfunctional and had higher hospitalization rates as well as lower global functioning scores compared with children diagnosed with pure ADHD.

Results of other studies are consistent with these high comorbid rates. Butler et al. (1995) reported that among hospitalized children with ADHD, depression co-occurred for 36%, bipolar disorder for 22%, affective psychosis for 8% and dysthymia was comorbid for 3%. Only 31% had no concurrent affective diagnosis.

Family studies indicate a genetic link between depression and ADHD. Biederman and colleagues (1992) reported that rates of this disorder within the families of ADHD children (with or without comorbid depression) were significantly higher than among relatives of control children. This suggests that for some children, a particular genetic factor may contribute to ADHD, whereas in others this same factor may contribute to depression or to the co-occurrence of both disorders.

ADHD with bipolar disorder. There are many challenging issues in the treatment of children with comorbid bipolar disorder and ADHD. According to Janet Wozniak (2001), the rate of bipolar disorder in children has been underestimated. As with depression, the presentation of the disorder in children is different than in adults and may be difficult to distinguish from normal developmental issues. Manic children tend to demonstrate irritability, anger, and oppositional or aggressive behavior rather than euphoria. Symptoms such as grandiosity, racing thoughts, and pressured speech are often present, however. Manic outbursts in children may be intense but be relatively short-lived and have been termed "affective storms" for these reasons (Davis, 1979).

Because of symptoms shared between mania and ADHD, it may be difficult to discern whether the child has bipolar disorder, ADHD, or both. Specifically, mania and ADHD in children overlap with respect to distractibility, physical hyperactivity, and talkativeness. Because children with non-comorbid ADHD do not manifest mood disturbances, the presence of mood instability suggests the presence of bipolar syndrome.

Fifty percent of prepubescent depressed children in one sample manifested symptoms of bipolar disorder within ten years of the onset of depression (Geller et al., 2001). In a study of children referred for a psychopharmacologic evaluation, 16% of the children less than twelve years of age met the criteria for mania (Wozniak et al., 1995). Only one child who met the criteria for mania failed to justify the diagnosis of ADHD. When comparing the children with ADHD without mania, the manic children had significantly higher rates of major depression, psychosis, multiple anxiety disorders, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder as well as significantly greater impairment in psychosocial functioning.

ADHD with substance abuse. With or without comorbidity, ADHD is a risk factor for the occurrence of substance abuse among adults (Biederman et al., 1998B; Wilens et al., 1997). When an individual presents with both substance abuse and ADHD, clinicians should first attempt to stabilize and treat the substance abuse disorder. Depression should be treated next, followed by treatment of the ADHD.

Substance-abusing patients treated with methylphenidate evidenced a reduction in ADHD symptoms as well as an improvement in cocaine craving in some studies (Levin et al., 1998) or no increase in craving in other studies (Grabowski et al., 1997). Of course, the care giver must consider the degree of abuse potential associated a drug when choosing the most appropriate medication for this group.

A common concern is that the treatment of children with stimulants will increase the rates of substance abuse over time. However, findings from retrospective and prospective studies do not support this hypothesis, particularly when controlling for baseline severity. In fact, successful treatment of ADHD in childhood or adolescence appears to have dampening effects with respect to the later development of substance abuse. In a four-year follow-up study of ADHD and non-ADHD families, Biederman and colleagues (1999) observed that subjects with untreated ADHD had much higher rates of later substance abuse than did both treated ADHD patients and controls. Also, there is evidence that untreated ADHD is associated with higher rates of alcohol use at 15-year follow-up (Loney, 1998). Thus, although ADHD and substance abuse are highly comorbid among adults, treatment of ADHD in children or adolescents may offer a degree of protection from later substance abuse.

Treatment in the medical and the social model

Abundant data bear on the approaches to treatment for children with ADHD. Because this disorder can have lifelong consequences, interventions are designed to reduce the negative outcomes and increase functional capacities.

Only three modes of treatment have received substantial and consistent support as effective short-term interventions for ADHD: behavior modification, stimulants, and a combination of the two. Despite the evidence on efficacy, relatively little is known about the degree to which these interventions are employed in clinical and educational practice or in the family setting. Ther is scant evidence regarding the effectiveness of these treatments in non-research settings. Pharmacologic treatment is prevalent and short-term use has been shown to be effective in reducing ADHD symptoms. The longer-term efficacy of behavioral and pharmacological treatments has not been firmly established.

In the US particularly, the prescribing of stimulants for ADHD has increased quite dramatically. The use of Ritalin grew by 500% from 1990 through May 1995 (Diller, 1996). Epidemiological studies estimated that the 12-month prescription prevalence for school-aged children was at 6% in urban areas (Safer, Zito & Fine, 1996). In another study, family physicians estimated that 6.9% of children seen in their practice were using medications to treat ADHD. This estimate made by pediatricians was 10.8% and child psychiatrists estimated 55.6%of children in their practice were using medication as treatment for ADHD (Sax & Kautz, 2003).

Medications used in the treatment of ADHD act by inhibiting the dopamine transporter and/or increasing the release of dopamine into the synaptic cleft. Medications that increase the production of norepinephrine also have had success in treating the symptoms of ADHD. Research documents the improvement of the core symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity. Effects on the outcomes of intelligence and achievement tests are less significant (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2001).

The purely medical model intervention of psychopharmacology was well-entrenched. However, as a result of research findings, multimodal treatment interventions, including aspects of both the medical and the social model, became the gold standard for treating ADHD. One of the most widely-cited studies was touched upon earlier in laying out the historic timeline of ADHD; this is the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (the MTA Study) which was co-sponsored by the US National Institute of Mental Health and the US Department of Education. The purpose of this study was to investigate four different treatment conditions: medication only; medication and psychosocial therapy; psychosocial therapy only; and community care. At the end of a 14-month treatment period, results indicated that medication intervention alone and combined medication and psychosocial therapy provided greater treatment efficacy than did psychosocial intervention alone or community care (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999). At the end of 24 months, 68% of the combined treatment group, 56% in the medication only group, 34% in the psychosocial only group and 25% in the community care group showed improvement (Swanson et al., 2001). This is one among many examples of empirical endorsements for multimodal interventions, leaving little doubt that the social worker has a primary role to play in the treatment of ADHD.

Behavioral problems leave a legacy of low self-esteem for the child and high levels of stress for the family (Sekelman & Snyder, 2000). The risk of a deficit in social skills is greater in children with learning or attention problems (Kavale & Forness, 1996). Behavioral modification therapy includes interventions used to modify the physical and the social environment in order to alter the child’s behavior. Strategies involving social skills training and/or cognitive behavioral therapy may be effective in helping the child with ADHD gain control over target behaviors and promote a mastery of everyday routines through organizational strategies (Stein, 2002).

Parent training in behavioral modification and classroom behavioral interventions have been shown to be effective in changing the disruptive behavior of children with ADHD. Research provides evidence of the positive treatment effects of parent training, including increases in child compliance, in the use of recommended parental commands/communications, in the knowledge of appropriate parenting techniques, and in positive parental statements (McGoey, Eckert & Dupaul, 2002) Preliminary research evidence suggests that once effective parenting patterns are established and maintained, positive effects in the behavior of preschool-age children with ADHD may be observed in settings outside of the home and over an expanse of time (McGoey et al., 2002).

Increasing the parent’s knowledge regarding ADHD may also increase parental satisfaction and appraisal of competency. According to empirical evidence, the level of parental knowledge about this disorder has an effect on choices regarding the child’s treatment and long-term compliance with a treatment plan (Flannagan, Pillow & Wise, 2002). Without the requisite knowledge, parents are more prone to seek from ‘the family doctor’ and to follow the GP’s recommendations, however partial or incorrect this treatment may be (Malacrida, 2001; Sayal et al., 2002). The social worker can intervene in these situations to educate and guide the parents to a more appropriate first contact for treatment. Reinforcing this social work role is the fact that, because ADHD has a genetic component, one or both parents may have the disorder, making it difficult for them to regulate their own behavior and to provide the systematic support that the child requires (Voeller, 2004).

An alternative intervention

There are myriad claims of alternative that intervention, touting cures for ADHD, as well as symptom reduction. One of these is the Dore Programme. Through a series of exercises, mainly eye, balancing, and coordination, this program claims to cure the problems in the person’s cerebellum that Dore claims to be causal of ADHD. Unfortunately, no systematic, independent, well-controlled experimentation is reported to lend credence to the effects of being anything more than a placebo effect, boosted by wishful thinking and the high price of participation.

Outcomes and Impact of ADHD

Given the nature of ADHD, it is believed to have a clear impact on social, economic, educational and health care delivery systems. The associated burden of ADHD impacts the individual experiencing the disorder; the family members and caregivers; the school system; employers; systems of care, including medical, mental health and social work professionals; insurers; the justice system; the community and society at large (Hinshaw, 1999). The burden may be associated with extremes in behavior and the ramifications of that behavior, as well as the deficits in skill attainment and the resulting implications for lost productivity. Documentation of the magnitude of the social and economic burden in these areas has been limited. Additionally, there is a dearth of evidence as to what types of intervention can be effective in improving the real-world outcomes for children with ADHD.

Effects on the Individual

Several studies have investigated the functional impairments associated with ADHD in childhood, especially if the disorder remains untreated. These children can have compromised academic achievement and school performance. Up to 80% demonstrate academic performance problems, with a significantly higher rate of being ‘held back’ (i.e., retained at the same grade level whilst peers are promoted), a higher rate of special education placement, of suspension from school and of dropping out when compared to their peers (Rabiner et al., 2004). This study also indicated that the percentage of students with inattentive subtype ADHD who rated below grade level ranged from 76% in reading to 92% below in written language. As many as half the children with ADHD are suspended from school as a result of impulsive acts. The prevalence of children in special education with ADHD and a specific learning disability was 65%, compared to 46% of children with a learning disability only. Among children with a diagnosis of learning disability, 54% were in special education. This was almost five times greater than the percent observed for children with ADHD only (Department of Health and Human Services, 2002).

A review of the literature also reveals that, in childhood and adolescents, ADHD clearly is associated with lowered self-esteem and marked difficulties with social relationships. Because a child’s self-image is dependent upon the success experienced in school and in relationships with parents and peers, it is not difficult to imagine the ways in which self-esteem may suffer for a child with this disorder.

Researchers have investigated self-esteem in children with ADHD and co-occurring problems with internalization and externalization. In a study by Bussing and colleagues (2000) findings suggested that ADHD alone does not appear to account for lowered self-esteem. It was the presence of comorbid internalization problem (mood or anxiety disorders) that accounted for the variance in self-esteem. Evidence suggests that difficulties with peer relationships predict a number of subsequent problems. Rejected children fare less well in adolescence and adulthood than do children who are able to establish harmonious relations with their peers. One explanation offered for this finding is that rejected children often gravitate toward one another, reinforcing and escalating antisocial behavior among the outcast group (Rabiner, 2001). Bagwell et al. (2001) reported that ADHD during childhood predicts later impairment in adolescents’ peer relationships, including the content of parents’ reports regarding their child’s close friendships and level of acceptance by peers.

Social difficulties may be explained as manifestations of social knowledge deficit (i.e., lacking the knowledge of ways to appropriately interact), or social performance deficits (i.e., having the knowledge of what to do, but lacking the ability to put that knowledge into action). With this dual explanation in mind, Maedgen and Carson (2000) investigated whether children with different subtypes of ADHD differ in the primary reason for their social difficulties. Children with combined type ADHD were less popular with peers according to parents and teachers, and were seen as less liked than were children with inattentive type ADHD. Children diagnosed with combined type ADHD were rated as more likely to enact aggressive behavior, thus exhibiting social performance deficits. Children in the inattentive type subgroup also exhibited social performance deficits and were more likely to respond in passive ways (i.e., just letting things go, without taking steps to deal with the situation). These children were found to be slightly less knowledgeable about socially appropriate ways to respond (deficit in social knowledge). Children classified in the combined subtype were more prone to intense displays of negative emotion and had difficulty regulating these emotions. Problems with emotion regulation could contribute to being disliked by peers. One limitation of the research on improving peer relationships for children with ADHD is the limited focus on improving a child’s overall standing within a peer group; no one has investigated ways to help that child make a single close friendship. The review of the literature demonstrates an increased risk for accidental injuries in children and adolescents with ADHD. Teens who are not treated have a significantly higher rate of traffic violations than do their peers (Barkley et al, 2002B). Those with significant ADHD symptoms were nearly three times more likely to be involved in an accident than were those whose symptoms were less significant; they were about 2.3 times more likely to report having driven while intoxicated, and were also more likely to have been cited for a traffic violation (Woodward, Ferguson & Horwood, 2000). A small study provided evidence that the controlled-release stimulant medications yielded substantial improvement in the driving performance of individuals with ADHD (Cox et al., 2004).

Other health risk behaviors identified as more like to occur n adolescents with ADHD include smoking, alcohol use, marijuana use and the use of some other illicit drugs. Researchers found that adolescents with a high level of ADHD symptomology were ten times more likely to have smoked, eight times more likely to report an urge to smoke, and almost four times more likely to have consumed alcohol (Whalen et al., 2002).

In addition, studies have examined predictors of substance abuse among adolescents who were diagnosed with ADHD in childhood. Inattentive symptoms as children more strongly predicted adolescent substance abuse than did hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. As a possible explanation for this obtained effect, researchers noted that attention problems are more strongly associated with academic failure than are impulse/hyperactivity symptoms. Ongoing academic struggles may lead a child away from conventional peers with an orientation toward academic success, and toward nonconformist peer groups where substance abuse may be more tolerated, modeled, and encouraged. Adolescents with persistent ADHD were more than twice as like to be smokers. Rates of other substance use problems were comparable to adolescents without ADHD. Adolescents with persistent ADHD who had developed a diagnosable conduct disorder had the highest rate of substance use problems (Molina & Pelham, 2003).

As previously mentioned, although parents may be concerned that starting a child on psychostimulant medication for the treatment of ADHD will increase the likelihood of subsequent drug abuse, available data suggest the opposite effect. A meta-analysis performed by Faraone & Wilens (2003) pooled data from six different studies, yielding the finding that children treated with stimulant medication were about half as likely as were other children to develop a substance abuse disorder later in life. In studies where medicated and non-medicated groups of children showed equivalent severity of symptoms prior to treatment, those who did not receive medication were over three times more likely to develop a substance abuse disorder.

The literature on functional impairment due to ADHD symptoms provides evidence for continuity of ADHD-related impairment across the lifespan, suggesting that related challenges faced by adolescents correlate with difficulties in adulthood. In the past decade, adults with ADHD have changed jobs more often that have individuals without the burden of this disorder. They are less likely to report good current relationships with parents and peers; 28% have undergone divorce compared with 15% of controls (Faraone & Biederman, 2005). In another study, rates of arrest (ADHD, combined type 40%; inattentive type 19%; and 13% in the control) and self-reported drug and alcohol use (80% in the combined type; 81% in inattentive; 52% in the control) were consistently higher among adults with ADHD (Murphy et al., 2002).

Conclusion: The Social Workers’ Place and Perspective

Empirical findings have made the Multimodal approach the gold standard for helping children with ADHD and the families of those children. The specific recommended interventions include psychostimulant medication; behavior management skills training for the children’s parents and their teachers; supportive, cognitive and play therapy to promote self-esteem development; and supportive therapy and education for parents.

The legislation with enabling funding (e.g., the Education Act, the Children’s Act 1989, Every Child Matters) reinforces the notion of a coordinated system of care, child-centered and multimodal. No profession is better suited than social work to advantage the funding opportunities while advocating for the welfare of the child and family within a multimodal treatment framework.

Effective work with parents of ADHD children requires the social worker to understand what this disability means to the family. Each ADHD child constitutes a particular and unique constellation of needs; understanding that requirement set and assessing the meaning of the child’s disability within that particular family is critical to the success of any subsequent intervention. Social workers are well-trained and experienced in this type of humane, although systematic, analysis of the situation and in action planning and coordination for the multimodal approach.

References

American Academy of Pediatrics. Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Committee on Quality Improvement (2001). Clinical practice guideline: Treatment of the school-aged child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 108(4), pp. 1033-1034.

Bagwell, C.L. (2001). ADHD and problems in peer relations. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, pp. 1285-1292.

Barkley, R.A., E.H. Cook, A. Diamond, et al. (2002). International consensus statement on ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5, 89 –111.

Barkley, R.A., K.R. Murphy, G.I. Dupaul & T. Bush (2002B). Driving in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Knowledge, performance, adverse outcomes, and the role of executive functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 8, pp. 655-672.

Biederman J., S.V. Faraone, K. Keenan, J. Benjamin, B. Kritcher, C. Moore, Sprich, S. Buckminster, K. Ugaglia, M.S. Jellinek & R. Steingard (1992). Further evidence for family-genetic risk factors in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Patterns of comorbidity in probands and relatives in psychiatrically and pediatrically referred samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 49, pp. 728-738.

Biederman J, S. Faraone, S. Milberger, J. Guite, E. Mick, L. Chen, D. Mennin, A. Marrs, C. Ouellette, P. Moore, T. Spencer, D. Norman, T. Wilens, I. Kraus & J. Perrin (1996). A prospective 4-year follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 53, pp. 437-446.

Biederman, J., E. Mick, J.Q. Bostic, J. Prince, J. Daly, T.E. Wilens, T. Spencer, J. Garcia-Jetton, R. Russell, J. Wozniak, S.V. Faraone (1998). The naturalistic course of pharmacologic treatment of children with maniclike symptoms: A systematic chart review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, pp. 628-637.

Biederman , J., E. Mick, S.V. Faraone, T. Spencer & T.E. Wilens (2000). Pediatric mania: A developmental subtype of bipolar disorder? Biological Psychiatry, 48, pp. 458-466.

Biederman, J., T.E. Wilens, E. Mick, S.V. Faraone & T. Spencer (1998B). Does attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder impact the developmental course of drug and alcohol abuse and dependence? Biol Psychiatry, 44, pp. 269-273.

Biederman, J., T. Wilens, E. Mick, T. Spencer & S.V. Faraone (1999). Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder reduces risk for substance use disorder. Pediatrics, 104(2), p. e20.

Bussing R, B.T. Zima, A.R. Perwien (2000). Self-esteem in special education children with ADHD: Relationship to disorder characteristics and medication use. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 39, pp. 1260-1269.

Coghill, D. (2005). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Should we believe the mass media or peer-reviewed literature? Psychiatric Bulletin, 29, 288-291.

Cox, D.J., R.L. Merkel & J.K. Penberthy, et al. (2004). Impact of methylphenidate delivery profiles on driving performance of adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(3), pp. 269-275.

Davis, R.E. (1979). Manic-depressive variant syndrome of childhood: a preliminary report. American J Psychiatry, 136, pp. 702-706.

Department of Health & Human Services (2002). Attention deficit disorder and learning disability: United States, 1997-98 (DHHS Publication No (PHS) 2002-1534). Hyattsville, MD: Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics.

Diller, L.H. (1996). The run on Ritalin: Attention deficit disorder and stimulant treatment in the 1990s. The Hastings Center Report, 26(2), pp. 12-18.

Eigenmann, P.A. & C.A. Haenggeli (2004). Food colourings and preservatives – allergy and hyperactivity. The Lancet, 364, pp. 823-824.

Faraone, S.V. (2001). Patterns of comorbidity in ADHD: artifact or reality? Program and abstracts of the 154th Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association; May 5-10, 2001; New Orleans, Louisiana. Industry Symposium 46B.

Faraone, S.V. & J. Biederman (2005). Adolescent predictors of functional outcome in adult ADHD: A population survey. Program and abstracts of the American Psychiatric Association Meeting 2005 Annual Meeting. Poster NR458.

Faraone, S.V. & T.E. Wilens (2003). Does stimulant treatment lead to substance abuse disorders? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, pp. 9-13.

Feingold, B.F. (1973). Food additives and child development. Hospital Practice, 21, pp. 11-18.

Flannagan, D., D.R. Pillow & J.P. Wise (2002). Perceptions and communications about ADHD and ODD behaviors in children with combined type attention deficit disorder. Children’s Health Care, 31(3), pp. 223-236.

Frith, U. (Ed.) (1991). Autism and Asperger syndrome. Cambridge UK: University Press.

Geller, B., B. Zimerman, M. Williams, K. Bolhofner, B.S. & J.L. Craney (2001). Bipolar disorder at prospective follow-up of adults who had prepubertal major depressive disorder. American J Psychiatry, 158, pp. 125-127.

Gillberg, C. (2003). Deficits in attention, motor control, and perception: a brief review. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 88, pp. 904-910.

Grabowski, J., J.D. Roache, J.M. Schmitz, H. Rhodes, D. Creson & A. Korszum (1997). Replacement medication for cocaine dependence: methylphenidate. J Clin Psychopharmacol, 17, pp. 485-488.

Hallowell, E.M. & J.R. Ratey (1995). Driven to Distraction: Recognizing and Coping with Attention Deficit Disorder from Childhood through Adulthood. New York, NY: Touchstone.

Hinshaw, S. P. (1999, September). Individual, social, and economic burden associated with ADHD. Invited address delivered at the Centers for Disease Control, Atlanta. http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/adhd/dadabbur.htm Date accessed: 18/02/2006.

James, A., F.H. Lai & C. Dahl (2004). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and suicide: A review of possible associations. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 110(6), pp. 408-415.

Kavale, K.A. & S.R. Forness (1996). Social skills deficits and learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, pp. 226-237.

Kellner, R., S. Houghton & G. Douglas (2003). Peer-related personal experiences of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with and without comorbid learning disabilities. International Journal of Disability, 50(2), pp. 119-136.

Klin, A. & F.R. Volkmar (1997). Asperger’s Syndrome. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.). Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders (2nd ed., pp. 94-122). New York: Wiley & Sons.

Lesesne, C.A., S.N. Visser & C.P. White (2003). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in school-aged children: Association with maternal mental health and use of health care resource. Pediatrics, 111(5), pp. 1232-1237.

Levin, F.R., S.M. Evans, D.M. McDowell & H.D. Kleber (1998). Methylphenidate treatment for cocaine abusers with adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a pilot study. J Clin Psychiatry, 59, pp. 300-305.

Loney, J. (1998). Substance abuse in adolescents: diagnostic issues from studies of attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. NIDA Res Monogr, 77, pp. 19-26.

Maedgen, J.W. & C.L. Carlson (2000). Social functioning and emotional regulation in the ADHD subtypes. Journal of Child Clinical Psychology, 29, pp. 30-42.

Malacrida, C. (2001). Motherhood, resistance and attention deficit disorder: Strategies and limits. The Canadian Review of Sociology & Anthropology, 38(2), pp. 141-165.

McCleary, L. (2002). Parenting adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Analysis of the literature for social work practice. Health & Social Work, 27(4), pp. 285-292.

McGoey, K.E., T.L. Eckert & G.J. Dupaul (2002). Early intervention with preschool-age children with ADHD: A literature review. Journal of Emotional & Behavioral Disorders, 10(1), p. 14.

Milberger, S., J. Biederman, S.V. Faraone, J. Murphy & M.T. Tsuang (1995). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and comorbid disorders: issues of overlapping symptoms. American J Psychiatry, 152, pp. 1793-1799.

Molina, B.S. & W.E Pelham (2003). Childhood predictors of adolescent substance abuse in a longitudinal study of children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(3), pp. 497-507.

MTA Cooperative Group (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 56(12), pp. 1073-1086.

Murphy, K, et al. (2002). Young adults with ADHD: Subtype differences in comorbidity, education, and clinical history. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 190, pp. 147-157.

Neufeld, P. & M. Foy (2006). Historical reflections on the ascendancy of ADHD in North America. British Journal of Educational Studies, 54(4), pp. 449-470.

Pineda, D.A., F. Lopera, J.D. Palacio, D. Ramirez & G. C. Henao (2003). Prevalence estimations of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: differential diagnoses and comorbidities in a Columbian sample. International Journal of Neuroscience, 113(1), pp. 49-71

Rabiner, D.L. (2001). Does ADHD in childhood predict peer problems in adolescence? Attention Research Update, 43.

Rabiner, D.L., D.W. Murray, L. Schmid & P. Malone, P. (2004). An exploration of the relationship between ethnicity, attention problems, and academic achievement, School Psychology Review, 33, pp. 498-509.

Remschmidt, H. (2005). Global consensus on ADHD/HKD. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 14(3), pp. 127-137.

Safer, D., J. Zito & E. Fine (1996). Increased methylphenidate usage for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in the 1990’s. Pediatrics, 98, pp. 1084-1088.

Sax, L. & K.J. Kautz (2003). Who first suggested the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Annals of Family Medicine, 3, pp. 171-174.

Sayal, K., E. Taylor, J. Beecham & P. Byrne (2002). Pathways to care in children at risk of attention-deficit disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, pp. 43-48.

Sekelman, J. & M. Snyder (2000). Learning disabilities and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In P.L. Jackson & J.A. Vessey (Eds.), Primary care of the child with a chronic disorder (pp. 631-657). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.

Stein, M.T. (2002). The role of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder diagnostic and treatment guidelines in changing physician practices. Pediatric Annals, 31, pp. 496-504.

Still, G.F. (1902). The Goulstonian lectures on some abnormal psychical conditions in childhood. Lectures 1, 2 & 3. Lancet., 1, pp. 1008–1012., 1077-82, 1163-68.

Swanson, J.M., H.C. Kraemer, S.P. Hinshaw, et al. (2001). Clinical relevance of the primary findings of the MTS: Success rates based on severity of ADHD and ODD symptoms at the end of treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, pp. 168-179.

Timimi, S. & E. Taylor (2004). ADHD is best understood as a cultural construct. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, pp. 8-9.

Troost, P.W., M-P Steenhuis, H.G. Tuynman-Qua, L.J. Kalverdijk, J.K. Buitelaar, R.B. Minderaa & P.J. Hoekstra (2006). Atomoxetine for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders: A pilot study. J of Child & Adol Psychopharmacology, 16(5), pp. 611-619.

Voeller, K.K. (2004). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Neurology, 19(10), pp. 798-814.

Whalen, C.K., Jamner, L.D., B. Henker, R.J. Delfino & J.M. Lozano (2002). The ADHD spectrum and everyday life: Experience sampling of adolescent moods, activities, smoking, and drinking. Child Development, 73(1), pp. 209-227.

Wilens, T.E., J. Biederman, E. Mick, S.V. Faraone & T. Spencer (1997). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with early onset substance use disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis, 185, pp. 475-482.

Wolraich, M.L. (2006). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Can it be recognized and treated in children younger than 5 years? Infants & Young Children, 19(2), pp. 86-93.

Woodward, L.J., D.M. Ferguson & L.J. Horwood (2000). Driving outcomes of young people with attentional difficulties in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(5), pp. 627-634.

Wozniak, J. (2001). Bipolar disorder and ADHD: an overlooked comorbidity. Program and abstracts of the 154th Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association; May 5-10, 2001; New Orleans, Louisiana. Industry Symposium 46D.

Wozniak, J., J. Biederman, K. Kiely, J.S. Ablon, S.V. Faraone, E. Mundy & D. Mennin (1995). Mania-like symptoms suggestive of childhood-onset bipolar disorder in clinically referred children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(7), pp. 867-76.

Bibliography

American Academy of Pediatrics. Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Committee on Quality Improvement (2001). Clinical practice guideline: Treatment of the school-aged child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 108(4), pp. 1033-1034.

Angello, L.M., R.J. Volpe, J.C. DiPerna, S.P. Gureasko-Moore, D.P. Gureasko-Moore, M.R. Nebrig & K. Ota (2003). Assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an evaluation of six published rating scales. School Psychology Review, 32(2), pp. 241- 262.

Altshuler, S.J. & S. Kopels (2003). Advocating in schools for children with disabilities: What’s new with IDEA? Social Work, 48(3), pp. 320-329.

Bagwell, C.L. (2001). ADHD and problems in peer relations. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, pp. 1285-1292.

Barkley, R.A., E.H. Cook, A. Diamond, et al. (2002). International consensus statement on ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5, 89 –111.

Barkley, R.A., K.R. Murphy, G.I. Dupaul & T. Bush (2002B). Driving in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Knowledge, performance, adverse outcomes, and the role of executive functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 8, pp. 655-672.

Berbatis, C.G., V.B. Sunderland & M. Bulsara (2002). Licit psychostimulant consumption in Australia, 1984-2000: International and jurisdictional comparison. Medical Journal of Australia, 177, pp. 539-542.

Bernier, J.C. & D.H. Siegel (1994). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A family and ecological systems perspective. Families in Society, 75(3), pp. 142-151.

Biederman J., S.V. Faraone, K. Keenan, J. Benjamin, B. Kritcher, C. Moore, Sprich, S. Buckminster, K. Ugaglia, M.S. Jellinek & R. Steingard (1992). Further evidence for family-genetic risk factors in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Patterns of comorbidity in probands and relatives in psychiatrically and pediatrically referred samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 49, pp. 728-738.

Biederman J, S. Faraone, S. Milberger, J. Guite, E. Mick, L. Chen, D. Mennin, A. Marrs, C. Ouellette, P. Moore, T. Spencer, D. Norman, T. Wilens, I. Kraus & J. Perrin (1996). A prospective 4-year follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 53, pp. 437-446.

Biederman, J., E. Mick, J.Q. Bostic, J. Prince, J. Daly, T.E. Wilens, T. Spencer, J. Garcia-Jetton, R. Russell, J. Wozniak, S.V. Faraone (1998). The naturalistic course of pharmacologic treatment of children with maniclike symptoms: A systematic chart review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, pp. 628-637.

Biederman , J., E. Mick, S.V. Faraone, T. Spencer & T.E. Wilens (2000). Pediatric mania: A developmental subtype of bipolar disorder? Biological Psychiatry, 48, pp. 458-466.

Biederman, J., T.E. Wilens, E. Mick, S.V. Faraone & T. Spencer (1998B). Does attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder impact the developmental course of drug and alcohol abuse and dependence? Biol Psychiatry, 44, pp. 269-273.

Biederman, J., T. Wilens, E. Mick, T. Spencer & S.V. Faraone (1999). Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder reduces risk for substance use disorder. Pediatrics, 104(2), p. e20.

Bramble, D. (2003). Annotation: The use of psychotropic medications in children: a British view. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 44(2), pp. 169-179.

Braun, J.M., R.S. Kahn, T. Froehlich, P. Auinger & B.P. Lanphear (2006). Exposures to environmental toxicants and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children. Environmental Health Perspectives, 114(12), pp. 1904-1909.

Brewis, A., K.L. Schmidt & C.A.S. Casas (2003). Cross-cultural study of the childhood developmental trajectory of attention and impulse control. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), pp. 174-181.

Brown, K. (2003). New attention to ADHD genes. Science, 301(5630), pp. 161-162.

Bussing, R., B.T. Zima, F.A. Gary & C.W. Garvan (2003). Barriers to detection, help-seeking, and service use for children with ADHD symptoms. Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research, 30(2), pp. 176-189.

Bussing R, B.T. Zima, A.R. Perwien (2000). Self-esteem in special education children with ADHD: Relationship to disorder characteristics and medication use. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 39, pp. 1260-1269.

Coghill, D. (2005). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Should we believe the mass media or peer-reviewed literature? Psychiatric Bulletin, 29, 288-291.

Cox, D.J., R.L. Merkel & J.K. Penberthy, et al. (2004). Impact of methylphenidate delivery profiles on driving performance of adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(3), pp. 269-275.

Davis, R.E. (1979). Manic-depressive variant syndrome of childhood: a preliminary report. American J Psychiatry, 136, pp. 702-706.

Department of Health & Human Services (2002). Attention deficit disorder and learning disability: United States, 1997-98 (DHHS Publication No (PHS) 2002-1534). Hyattsville, MD: Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics.

Diller, L.H. (1996). The run on Ritalin: Attention deficit disorder and stimulant treatment in the 1990s. The Hastings Center Report, 26(2), pp. 12-18.

dosReis, S., J.M. Zito, D.J. Safer & K.L. Soeken (2001). Mental health services for youths in foster care and disabled youths. American Journal of Public Health, 91(7), pp. 1094-1099.

Eigenmann, P.A. & C.A. Haenggeli (2004). Food colourings and preservatives – allergy and hyperactivity. The Lancet, 364, pp. 823-824.

Faraone, S.V. (2001). Patterns of comorbidity in ADHD: artifact or reality? Program and abstracts of the 154th Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association; May 5-10, 2001; New Orleans, Louisiana. Industry Symposium 46B.

Faraone, S.V. & J. Biederman (2005). Adolescent predictors of functional outcome in adult ADHD: A population survey. Program and abstracts of the American Psychiatric Association Meeting 2005 Annual Meeting. Poster NR458.

Faraone, S.V., J. Sargeant, C. Gillberg & J. Biederman (2003). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: Is it an American condition? World Psychiatry, 2(2), pp. 104-113.

Faraone, S.V. & T.E. Wilens (2003). Does stimulant treatment lead to substance abuse disorders? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, pp. 9-13.

Feingold, B.F. (1973). Food additives and child development. Hospital Practice, 21, pp. 11-18.

Flannagan, D., D.R. Pillow & J.P. Wise (2002). Perceptions and communications about ADHD and ODD behaviors in children with combined type attention deficit disorder. Children’s Health Care, 31(3), pp. 223-236.

Frazier, T.W., E.A. Youngstrom, J.J. Glutting & M. W. Watkins (2007). ADHD and achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and sa concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(1), pp. 49-65.

Frith, U. (Ed.) (1991). Autism and Asperger syndrome. Cambridge UK: University Press.

Furman, L. (2005). What is attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? Journal of Child Neurology, 20(12), pp. 994-1002.

Frances, C., G. Hoizey, H. Millart & T. Trenque (2004). Paediatric methylphenidate (Ritalin) restrictive conditions of prescription in France. Briti



rev

Our Service Portfolio

jb

Want To Place An Order Quickly?

Then shoot us a message on Whatsapp, WeChat or Gmail. We are available 24/7 to assist you.

whatsapp

Do not panic, you are at the right place

jb

Visit Our essay writting help page to get all the details and guidence on availing our assiatance service.

Get 20% Discount, Now
£19 £14/ Per Page
14 days delivery time

Our writting assistance service is undoubtedly one of the most affordable writting assistance services and we have highly qualified professionls to help you with your work. So what are you waiting for, click below to order now.

Get An Instant Quote

ORDER TODAY!

Our experts are ready to assist you, call us to get a free quote or order now to get succeed in your academics writing.

Get a Free Quote Order Now